5.2
knowledge of sentence structure
Knowing which strings of words are grammatical and which are not is
part of syntactic knowledge. In addition, native speakers know at least
the following:
· Structural ambiguity --- which strings of words have more than one
meaning. For example, English speakers know that I like green
grapes and pears can be interpreted in two ways.
· Word order --- different arrangements of the same words have different
meanings.
· Grammatical relations --- what element relates to what other element
directly or indirectly. For example, in The boats are not big enough
and We have not enough boats, the word enough is related to
different words in the two sentences.
· Recursion --- the repeated use of the same rule(s) to create infinite
sentences. You are happy. I know that you are happy. He knows
that I know that you are happy. The formation of the last two of
these three sentences is based on the same rule. The rule can be applied
again and
again.
· Sentence relatedness --- sentences may be structurally variant but
semantically related.
· Syntactic categories --- a class of words or phrases that can substitute
for one another without loss of grammaticality. Consider these sentences:
(21) The child found the knife.
(22) A policeman found the knife.
(23) The man who just left here found the knife.
(24) He found the knife.
All the italicized parts belong to the same syntactic category called
noun phrase (NP). The noun phrases in these sentences function as subject.
The knife, also a noun phrase, functions as object. The above
listed aspects of syntactic knowledge, in addition to knowledge of grammaticality,
are implicit knowledge of English speakers. They know these subconsciously.
An English-speaking child accumulates all these aspects of knowledge unconsciously.
The task of the linguist is to explicate the knowledge, i.e. to spell
out the rules explicitly. The rest of this chapter will illustrate how
linguists manage to do this difficult job.
|