您现在的位置:首页>>英语泛读教程四>>UNIT 10

                                        

Exercises

Pay Attention, Rover  

by Amy Davis Mozdy

 

8:15 a.m. A flight lands at Melbourne's Tulamarine International Airport. Several hundred pieces of baggage are rushed from the plane onto a conveyor belt in the baggage reclaim annex. Over the sound of roaring engines, rushing air vents, and grinding generators, a dog barks, Florence, a sleek black Labrador, wags her tail.

Among the cavalcade of luggage passing beneath Florence's all-smelling nose is a nondescript hardback suitcase. Inside the case, within Styrofoam casing, packed in loose pepper and coffee, wrapped in freezer paper, and heat-sealed in plastic, are 18 kilograms of hashish.

The cleverly concealed drugs don't fool supersniffer Florence, and her persistent scratching at the case alerts her handler. Florence is one of a truly new breed: the product of what is perhaps the only project in the world dedicated to breeding dogs solely to detect drugs. Ordinary dogs have a 0.1 percent chance of making it in drug detection. The new breeding programme, run by the Australian Customs Service, is so successful that more than 50 percent of its dogs make the grade.

And what began as a wholly practical exercise in keeping illegal drugs out of Australia may end up playing a role in an entirely different sphere—the comparatively esoteric world of neurobiology. It turns out that it's not Florence's nose that makes her a top drug dog, but her unswerving concentration, plus a few other essential traits. Florence and her relatives could help neurobiologists to understand both what they call "attentional processing", the brain mechanisms that determine what a person pays attention to and for how long, and its flip side, problems such as attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder.

The Australian Customs Service has used dogs to find drugs since 1969. Traditionally, the animals came from pounds and private breeders. But in 1993, fed up with the poor success rate of finding good dogs this way, John Vandeloo, senior instructor with the detector dog unit, joined forces with Kath Champness, then a doctoral student at the University of Melbourne, and set up a breeding programme.

Champness, now with the Royal Guide Dogs Association of Australia, began by defining six traits that make a detector dog. "First, every good detector dog must love praise," she says, because this is the only tool the trainers have at their disposal. Then the dog needs a strong hunting instinct, and the strength to keep sniffing at the taxing rate of around 300 times per minute. The ideal detector is also fearless enough to deal with jam-packed airport crowds, and the roaring engine rooms of cargo ships.

Ivan the terrible

The remaining two traits are closely related and cognitive in nature. A good detector must be capable of focusing on the task of searching for drugs, despite the circus of distractions in any airport or dockside. This is what neurobiologists call "selective attention". And finally, with potentially tens of thousands of hiding places for drugs, the dog must persevere and maintain focus for hours at a time. Neurobiologists call this "sustained attention".

To create the supersniffer, Champness selected for these traits over three generations of dogs. She also discovered that of the six traits, selective attention was the most heritable. This finding, which Champness intends to publish later this year, shows that genes account for about 25 percent of the difference between dogs in their ability to pay attention. That's about as heritable as it gets for a complex behaviour in animals, according to Robert Plomin, deputy director of the Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Research Centre at the Institute of Psychiatry in London.

After only three generations, "the success of the breeding programme jumped way beyond our expectations," says Champness. "We hoped that 30 percent of the pups from the programme would become detector dogs, but the actual success rate consistently exceeds 50 percent."

Vandeloo and Champness assess the dog's abilities to concentrate by marking them on a scale between one and five according to how well they remain focused on a toy tossed into a patch of tall grass. Ivan scores a feeble one. He follows the toy, gets halfway there, then becomes distracted by places where other dogs have peed or by flowers in the pad dock. Rowena, on the other hand, has phenomenal concentration: some might even consider her obsessive. When Vandeloo tosses the toy, nothing can distract her from the searching, not other dogs, not food. And even if no one is around to encourage her, she keeps looking just the same. Rowena gets a five.

A person's ability to pay attention, like a dog's, depends on a number of overlapping cognitive behaviours, including memory and learning—the neurobiologists' attentional processing.

Attention in humans can be tested by asking subjects to spot colours on a screen while ignoring shapes, or to spot sounds while ignoring visual cues, or to take a "vigilance test". Sitting a vigilance test is like being a military radar operator. Blips appear on a cluttered monitor infrequently, and at irregular intervals. Rapid detection of all blips earns a high score. Within about five minutes, one in ten subjects will start to miss the majority of the blips, one in ten will still be able to spot nearly all of them, and the rest will come somewhere in-between.

Vigilance tasks push the limits of attention by providing signals that are infrequent and unpredictable—which is exactly what is expected of the detector dogs when they are asked to notice just a few odour molecules in the air, and then to home in on the source. During a routine mail screen that can take hours, the dogs stay so focused that not even a postcard lined with 0.5 grams of heroin and hidden in a bulging sack of letters escapes detection.

Dogged focus

By combining attention tests with brain scanning techniques in normal people, and in people with brain damage due to strokes and bullets, neurobiologists have identified which bits of the brain are activated when a person pays attention. Others have investigated what happens to the ability of a rodent or a monkey to focus when parts of their brains are lesioned, or when they are given drugs that block or enhance the effect of different neurotransmitters.

Gradually a coherent picture is emerging. Several areas in the brain's prefrontal cortex are especially important for attentional processing. Trevor Robins, an experiment psychologist at the University of Cambridge, explains that in humans "when a task gets difficult, the anterior cingulate gyrus [in the frontal lobe of the brain] seems to work overtime". The prefrontal cortex receives and sends inputs to most of the rest of the cortex which, among other tasks, processes visual and auditory information and language. But the prefrontal cortex alone is responsible for "executive" functions such as strategic planning and the allocation of cognitive resources.

The prefrontal cortex may even be able to boost the supply of stimulatory chemicals that reach the cortex during prolonged periods of concentration, by calling on parts of the brainstem and the basal forebrain, two areas that house the body's arousal centres and are located below the cortex. The prefrontal cortex provides the lion's share of the brain's neuronal input to these areas, and the whole cortex receives a lot of stimulation from the brainstem and the basal forebrain via neurons that feed its neurotransmitters like noradrenaline and acetylcholine. As Robbins says, through such mechanisms the prefrontal cortex is in a position to regulate the activity of the cortex—"it may turn the volume up and down".

Although the prefrontal cortex is obviously vital to an animal's ability to focus its attention, researchers are divided over the exact nature of its role. Neuroscientists like Raja Parasuraman argue that there are discrete control centres for attentional processing that reside in the prefrontal cortex. Parasuraman says that the prefrontal cortex decides what needs to be attended to next and what must be ignored.

Others, like Robbins, envisage a more democratic mechanism. They argue that attentional processing is the sum of a multitude of different mental processes including, say, moving to focus the eyes or ears on a target, and feeling enthusiastic or indifferent about a topic. Rather than a central control, they envisage an integrated network of processing carried out by different brain regions that, in addition to the cortex, include the amygdala, which plays a role in emotional responses, and the basal ganglia, which govern voluntary movement.

"Memory [also] plays an absolutely critical and essential role in guiding attention," says Robert Desimone, who studies the link between memory and attention, and has helped champion the idea that the attentional processing control centre lies in the prefrontal cortex.

Take the example of someone looking for a lost of keys, he says. The search begins in the prefrontal cortex, where neurons process the goal "find keys". These neurons make connection with the visual cortex at the back of the brain, activating a mental image of keys. In order to stay focused on the task at hand, the key image must have priority over all other images already stored in the memory.Desmone's work on monkeys suggests that the activity in the prefrontal and visual cortex which creates the image of keys also inhibits other neuronal connections that would conjure up distracting images.

"The end result is conscious perceptual awareness of the thing that is important right then, and unawareness of the things that aren't important," says Desimone.

Although most people have a degree of conscious control over how much attention they will pay to a given object or task, it constantly risks being hijacked by environmental stimuli. The search for lost keys, for example, is driven by the goal of finding the keys, or "top-down" voluntary control. But a cue from the environment, a knock at the door or the phone ringing, is likely to steer attention toward the door and away from the search. Such "bottom-up" effects are based solely on the properties of the stimulus itself and the brain's involuntary, instinctive response, and has nothing to do with attaining a goal.

Good detector dogs excel at top-down control. Training takes advantage of that and creates the goal by getting the dogs to associate finding a toy that smells like cocaine, hash or heroin, with praise from their trainers. Dogs like Ivan, on the other hand, are all bottom up.

Ivan is a bit like a person with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Children with ADHD are so distractable and their attention spans are so short that they can't function normally or learn properly. As many as 3 to 5 per cent of children are thought to suffer from the disease in the US, where the incidence is highest, although the diagnosis is often controversial.

    Wandering minds

In the past three years, ADHD researchers have begun to look for genetic components to the disorder. Plomin says that the studies "consistently and surprisingly show very substantial genetic influences—in the order of 60 per cent heritability". Behavioural traits in humans typically show higher heritabilities than they do in animals, perhaps because they can be characterised more easily in humans. Nonetheless, a 60 per cent heritability for ADHD is considered high because the disorder is likely to be a range of different defects that come under one umbrella diagnosis.

With the current interest in attentional processing, as well as human diseases that have an attention defect component, such as ADHD and schizophrenia (sufferers appear to lose voluntary control over where they focus their attention), Plomin predicts that it is only a matter of time before the super-sniffer dogs attract the attention of neurobiologists.

"At the behavioural level," says Plomin, "humans are much closer to dogs than to [rodents]." Rats are one of the favourite animals in which to study attentional processing. "Dogs are [also] more trainable than [rodents]," he points out. With their highly-heritable dynamo concentration, the Australian drug detection dogs could even help to identify some of the many genes that underlie attentional processing.

But so far, says Champness, nobody has beaten down her door with requests for dogs to study. Not that Melbourne's super-detector dogs aren't still very useful. During their first year of operation, the dogs made 1 500 drug seizures, including one 5-tonne load of cannabis in a shipping container and a smaller load concealed in condoms inside a smuggler's stomach.

(2 050 words)

(From New Scientist, 10 May 1997 )

 Text

Follow-up Exercises

A. Comprehending the text.

Choose the best answer.

1. This article focuses on ________. ( )

(a) how drug detector dogs are trained

(b) how cleverly drug detector dogs fulfill their duties

(c) a breeding programme of drug detector dogs

(d) analysis of attentional processing, with drug detector dogs as samples

2. ________ account(s) for Florence's success in detecting drugs. ( )

(a) The ability to concentrate

(b) Her sense of smell

(c) Being a new breed

(d) Six essential traits including "selective attention" and "sustained attention"

3. The significance of drug detector dogs in neurobiology mainly lies in ________. ( )

(a) helping neurobiologists detecting drugs' ingredients

(b) furthering understanding of attentional processing and its flip side

(c) exploring dogs' brain mechanism

(d) helping neurobiologists research on animal's attention deficit

4. Regarding the breeding programme, which statement is NOT true ? ( )

(a) It was set up to raise and train super drug detector dogs.

(b) It was set up because the traditional way to find qualified drug detector dogs didn't work well

(c) It successfully bred over 50% of the pups to be detector dogs, which was beyond the raisers' expectation.

(d) It assessed a dog's ability while it was playing toys.

5. According to Champness' discovery, among the six essential traits that make a detector dog, ________ is the most heritable, which shows ________ play(s) an important role in dogs concentration ability. ( )

(a) sustained attention ... genes

(b) praise-loving ...  mind

(c) selective attention ...  genes

(d) selective attention ...  mind

6. Vigilance test ________. ( )

(a) is adopted to monitor the attentional process

(b) provides infrequent and unnoticeable blips at regular intervals to test a person's ability to pay attention

(c) makes out a dog's attention ability by providing infrequent or noticeable signals

(d) is strictly based on the principle of radar

    7. Prefrontal cortex is especially important for attentional processing because ________. ( )

(a) it processes visual and auditory information and language alone

(b) it can regulate the activity of the cortex

(c) it is located below the cortex

(d) it is agreed unanimously that it is prefrontal cortex that decides what needs to be attended to next

   8. Desimone's example of someone looking for the lost keys is taken to show that ________. ( )

(a) attentional processing control centre resides in the prefrontal cortex

(b) prefrontal cortex alone is responsible for executive functions

(c) environmental stimuli can do nothing to affect prefrontal cortex

(d) the degree of conscious control varies for each person

  9. According to the article, which of the following is an example of "bottom-up" effect? ( )

(a) A dog concentrated on finding the toy without being distracted by other dogs or food .

(b) A student engaged in working out a math problem paused for a sudden sound.

(c) A man kept searching for lost keys but was knocked down by a wall.

(d) A boy listened to music while reading a novel.

  10. According to the article, which of the following is true? ( )

(a) ADHD is a disease of attention deficit / hyperactivity disorder, and can be definitely diagnosed.

(b) Super-detector dogs have been largely studied by neurobiologists.

(c) Behavioural traits in humans are more heritable than in animals.

(d) Super-detector dogs could help to identify all the genes that underlie attentional processing.

 

B. Topics for discussion.

1. What role can drug-detector dogs play in neurobiology?

 

2. What's the significance of the prefrontal cortex to an animal's ability to focus its attention?

 

                       

Text Exercises

 

©2006 高等教育出版社版权所有 (屏幕分辨率:800*600)